Differences
This shows you the differences between two versions of the page.
Both sides previous revision Previous revision Next revision | Previous revision | ||
group_1_presentation_3_-_crying [2018/03/30 18:49] asafa |
group_1_presentation_3_-_crying [2018/03/30 20:04] (current) mohameah |
||
---|---|---|---|
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
- | =======Crying Presentation Slides======= | + | |
+ | ====== Crying ====== | ||
+ | |||
+ | <box 50% round | > {{ :crying.jpg?600 |}} </box| Figure 1: Baby crying. https://www.ctvnews.ca/health/babies-in-canada-u-k-and-italy-cry-more-than-elsewhere-study-1.3352705 > | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | =====Presentation Slides==== | ||
{{:crying_slides.pdf|}} | {{:crying_slides.pdf|}} | ||
+ | |||
====== What is Crying? ====== | ====== What is Crying? ====== | ||
Crying is defined as the shedding of tears in response to an emotional state or physical irritation of the eye (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). Crying has been defined as a complex secremotor phenomenon that is characterized by the shedding of tears from the lacrimal apparatus, without any irritation of the ocular structures (Newman, 2007). It is often accompanied by alterations in the muscles that regulate facial expression and vocalization (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). For crying to be described as sobbing, it needs to be accompanied by a set of other symptoms such as: slow but erratic inhalation, occasional instances of breath holding and spasms of the respiratory and truncal muscle groups (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). | Crying is defined as the shedding of tears in response to an emotional state or physical irritation of the eye (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). Crying has been defined as a complex secremotor phenomenon that is characterized by the shedding of tears from the lacrimal apparatus, without any irritation of the ocular structures (Newman, 2007). It is often accompanied by alterations in the muscles that regulate facial expression and vocalization (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). For crying to be described as sobbing, it needs to be accompanied by a set of other symptoms such as: slow but erratic inhalation, occasional instances of breath holding and spasms of the respiratory and truncal muscle groups (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). | ||
+ | |||
===== Functions of Crying ===== | ===== Functions of Crying ===== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | <box 30% round right | > {{ :empathy.jpg?250 |}} </box| Figure 2: Empathy. https://mankindprojectjournal.org/2016/10/why-do-leaders-lack-empathy/ | ||
+ | > | ||
+ | |||
Crying can be elicited by various events that range from extremely negative to extremely positive experiences (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). Crying often occurs in situations that are characterized by separation, loss and helplessness and being overwhelmed by strong emotions. Crying serves two broad functions: intra-individual functions and inter-individual functions. Intra-individual functions of crying refer to the effects that crying has for the individual. These functions are predominantly linked to stress reduction and the experience of mood enhancement and relief that follows crying. Conversely, inter-individual functions of crying involve the effects that crying has on other people. Theories that analyze the social effects of crying stress the signal value of distress vocalizations and/or of human tears. From the attachment theory perspective, crying is viewed as an appeal for the presence and attention of the caregiver. Crying, particularly visible tears, has been shown to promote empathy and prosocial behaviour, facilitates social bonding and reduces inter-personal aggression. Recent research has shown that visible tears impact the evaluation of a human face and identify the need for support. On the other hand, the sound of infants crying has been shown to elicit anger, irritation, frustration and even aggression from others (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). | Crying can be elicited by various events that range from extremely negative to extremely positive experiences (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). Crying often occurs in situations that are characterized by separation, loss and helplessness and being overwhelmed by strong emotions. Crying serves two broad functions: intra-individual functions and inter-individual functions. Intra-individual functions of crying refer to the effects that crying has for the individual. These functions are predominantly linked to stress reduction and the experience of mood enhancement and relief that follows crying. Conversely, inter-individual functions of crying involve the effects that crying has on other people. Theories that analyze the social effects of crying stress the signal value of distress vocalizations and/or of human tears. From the attachment theory perspective, crying is viewed as an appeal for the presence and attention of the caregiver. Crying, particularly visible tears, has been shown to promote empathy and prosocial behaviour, facilitates social bonding and reduces inter-personal aggression. Recent research has shown that visible tears impact the evaluation of a human face and identify the need for support. On the other hand, the sound of infants crying has been shown to elicit anger, irritation, frustration and even aggression from others (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). | ||
====== Non-Humans Animals and Crying ====== | ====== Non-Humans Animals and Crying ====== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | <box 35% round | > {{ :wolforfox.jpg?300 |}} </box| Figure 3: A coyote howls. http://www.wideopenspaces.com/4-coyote-calls-need-know/ | ||
+ | > | ||
+ | |||
Humans have been identified as the only species to produce emotional tears. In all mammals and most birds, offspring react with separation calls or distress calls upon being removed from their parents (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). This is the phylogenetic basis of acoustical crying of human infants. This form of crying is meant to alleviate separation from the parents. In animals, distress calls are mainly displayed by young offspring and they are never accompanied by the production of tears. They are able to produce tears in response to irritation of the eye, or to keep the eyes moist, however, they do not cry in response to a psychological event (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). | Humans have been identified as the only species to produce emotional tears. In all mammals and most birds, offspring react with separation calls or distress calls upon being removed from their parents (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). This is the phylogenetic basis of acoustical crying of human infants. This form of crying is meant to alleviate separation from the parents. In animals, distress calls are mainly displayed by young offspring and they are never accompanied by the production of tears. They are able to produce tears in response to irritation of the eye, or to keep the eyes moist, however, they do not cry in response to a psychological event (Gracanin, Bylsma & Vingerhoets, 2014). | ||
Line 17: | Line 37: | ||
===== Distress Calls in Mammals ===== | ===== Distress Calls in Mammals ===== | ||
- | <box 30% round right | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.23.23_pm.png?300|}} </box| Figure 1: Distress calls in mammals. http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-2760764/Mammals-respond-distress-calls-newborns-regardless-species.html | + | <box 35% round right | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.23.23_pm.png?325|}} </box| Figure 4: Distress calls in mammals. http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-2760764/Mammals-respond-distress-calls-newborns-regardless-species.html |
> | > | ||
It has been shown that mammals respond to the distress calls of infants, regardless of species (Lingle & Riede, 2014). Researchers wanted to test the theory that a mammal mother’s instinct can recognise calls of any infant, regardless of species. They recorded the cries of various infant mammals. This included humans, fur seals, cats, dogs, young yellow-bellied marmots, sea lions, domestic cats, silver-haired bats and several other deer species. These recordings were played using hidden speakers to wild female mule and white-tailed deers in Canada. The calls were played at both normal and pitched frequencies and a positive response was recorded of the deer approached the speaker, which is similar to how they would react if their own fawn was crying. In experiments, wild mule and white-tailed deers responded equally to the cries of their own fawns as well as those of humans, cats and even infant silver-haired bats. However, they did not approach the speaker when the pitch fell outside the range of her fawn’s call. The researchers wanted to understand if the deers were simply responding to pitch rather than the type of call, so they played generic noises at frequencies outside the range of their fawn’s call. In all instances, the female deer did not respond to the control sounds, indicating that they were able to distinguish between the two types of noises. The results of this experiment suggest that the acoustic traits of infant distress vocalizations are essential for a response by caregivers and the caregivers sensitivity to those acoustic traits may be shared across diverse mammals. Furthermore, the frequency of distress calls across mammals likely differs to make it easier for species to identify each other (Lingle & Riede, 2014). | It has been shown that mammals respond to the distress calls of infants, regardless of species (Lingle & Riede, 2014). Researchers wanted to test the theory that a mammal mother’s instinct can recognise calls of any infant, regardless of species. They recorded the cries of various infant mammals. This included humans, fur seals, cats, dogs, young yellow-bellied marmots, sea lions, domestic cats, silver-haired bats and several other deer species. These recordings were played using hidden speakers to wild female mule and white-tailed deers in Canada. The calls were played at both normal and pitched frequencies and a positive response was recorded of the deer approached the speaker, which is similar to how they would react if their own fawn was crying. In experiments, wild mule and white-tailed deers responded equally to the cries of their own fawns as well as those of humans, cats and even infant silver-haired bats. However, they did not approach the speaker when the pitch fell outside the range of her fawn’s call. The researchers wanted to understand if the deers were simply responding to pitch rather than the type of call, so they played generic noises at frequencies outside the range of their fawn’s call. In all instances, the female deer did not respond to the control sounds, indicating that they were able to distinguish between the two types of noises. The results of this experiment suggest that the acoustic traits of infant distress vocalizations are essential for a response by caregivers and the caregivers sensitivity to those acoustic traits may be shared across diverse mammals. Furthermore, the frequency of distress calls across mammals likely differs to make it easier for species to identify each other (Lingle & Riede, 2014). | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | |||
====== Mechanism of Crying ====== | ====== Mechanism of Crying ====== | ||
Line 28: | Line 53: | ||
==== Lacrimal System ==== | ==== Lacrimal System ==== | ||
- | <box 30% round right | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.07.02_pm.png?250|}} </box| Figure 2: Lacrimal system. https://www.dreamstime.com/royalty-free-stock-photo-lacrimal-apparatus-image9845505 > | + | <box 30% round right | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.07.02_pm.png?350|}} </box| Figure 5: Lacrimal system. https://www.dreamstime.com/royalty-free-stock-photo-lacrimal-apparatus-image9845505 > |
The lacrimal system is named after its largest and main gland, called the lacrimal gland, which is responsible for expelling tears through a multitude of ducts around the eye (Botelho, 1964). The lacrimal gland is also commonly referred to as the tear duct. This system is composed of both a secretory and an excretory system, which produces tears and drains the tears, respectively. Tears are produced by the lacrimal gland, which are then secreted through ducts onto the surface of the eye. Some of the liquid evaporates between blinks, and some is drained through the lacrimal punctum, which will eventually pass through the nose. Any excess fluid that was not subject to evaporation or drainage through the punctum will fall over the eyelid, which in turn presents as the tears that an individual cries (Lutz, 1999). The lacrimal system does not solely synthesize and secrete tears, but also collects them (Botelho, 1964). As an individual blinks, the eyelids direct tear fluid towards the punctum. The punctum then acts as a passageway for the tears to move into the lacrimal sac, and subsequently downwards into the nasolacrimal duct (Botelho, 1964). This can explain why when a person is crying, they may also experience a runny nose. | The lacrimal system is named after its largest and main gland, called the lacrimal gland, which is responsible for expelling tears through a multitude of ducts around the eye (Botelho, 1964). The lacrimal gland is also commonly referred to as the tear duct. This system is composed of both a secretory and an excretory system, which produces tears and drains the tears, respectively. Tears are produced by the lacrimal gland, which are then secreted through ducts onto the surface of the eye. Some of the liquid evaporates between blinks, and some is drained through the lacrimal punctum, which will eventually pass through the nose. Any excess fluid that was not subject to evaporation or drainage through the punctum will fall over the eyelid, which in turn presents as the tears that an individual cries (Lutz, 1999). The lacrimal system does not solely synthesize and secrete tears, but also collects them (Botelho, 1964). As an individual blinks, the eyelids direct tear fluid towards the punctum. The punctum then acts as a passageway for the tears to move into the lacrimal sac, and subsequently downwards into the nasolacrimal duct (Botelho, 1964). This can explain why when a person is crying, they may also experience a runny nose. | ||
Line 37: | Line 62: | ||
There are three types of tears: basal tears, reflexive tears, and psychic tears (Lutz, 1999). Basal tears are synthesized at a rate of one to two microliters per minute. Their role is to keep the eye lubricated and regulate any abnormalities on the corneal surface (Lutz, 1999). The production of basal tears maintains a liquid film on the surface of the eye (Botelho, 1964). Reflexive tears are synthesized in response to irritants that come in contact or are around the eye, such physically getting poked in the eye or while chopping onions (Lutz, 1999). They are produced by the excitation of sensory receptors that are positioned in the conjunctiva of the eye and mucous membranes of the nose (Botelho, 1964). Psychic, or emotional, tears are produced by the lacrimal system and are the tears excreted during periods of intense emotion (Lutz, 1999). The lacrimal gland is fundamentally in charge of making reflexive and emotional tears. Interestingly, in a study conducted by Saree Penbharkkul and Samuel Karelitz, it was exhibited that the lacrimal system is not fully developed when a human is born (Botelho, 1964). Only approximately 13% of infants are able to produce tears when crying in the first five days of life. They suggested that this delayed capacity for tear production may be attributed to the impartial development of the parts of the central nervous system that innervate the lacrimal system in newborns (Botelho, 1964). | There are three types of tears: basal tears, reflexive tears, and psychic tears (Lutz, 1999). Basal tears are synthesized at a rate of one to two microliters per minute. Their role is to keep the eye lubricated and regulate any abnormalities on the corneal surface (Lutz, 1999). The production of basal tears maintains a liquid film on the surface of the eye (Botelho, 1964). Reflexive tears are synthesized in response to irritants that come in contact or are around the eye, such physically getting poked in the eye or while chopping onions (Lutz, 1999). They are produced by the excitation of sensory receptors that are positioned in the conjunctiva of the eye and mucous membranes of the nose (Botelho, 1964). Psychic, or emotional, tears are produced by the lacrimal system and are the tears excreted during periods of intense emotion (Lutz, 1999). The lacrimal gland is fundamentally in charge of making reflexive and emotional tears. Interestingly, in a study conducted by Saree Penbharkkul and Samuel Karelitz, it was exhibited that the lacrimal system is not fully developed when a human is born (Botelho, 1964). Only approximately 13% of infants are able to produce tears when crying in the first five days of life. They suggested that this delayed capacity for tear production may be attributed to the impartial development of the parts of the central nervous system that innervate the lacrimal system in newborns (Botelho, 1964). | ||
- | <box 40% round | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.15.02_pm.png?350|}} </box| Figure 3: Types of tears. https://steemit.com/news/@jonathanxvi/why-do-we-cry-the-three-types-of-tears > | + | <box 50% round | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.15.02_pm.png?525|}} </box| Figure 6: Types of tears. https://steemit.com/news/@jonathanxvi/why-do-we-cry-the-three-types-of-tears > |
==== Neurological System ==== | ==== Neurological System ==== | ||
- | |||
- | <box 35% round left | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.26.00_pm.png?250 |}} </box| Figure 4: Neurologic system. https://study.com/academy/lesson/cranial-nerves-of-the-face-and-mouth-motion-and-sensation-functionality.html > | ||
The trigeminal nerve, also known as the fifth cranial nerve, contains the sensory pathway that is responsible for tears (‘Tears’, 2018). The motor pathway is involuntary, and considered to be a part of the autonomic nervous system. This utilizes the pathway of the facial, or seventh cranial nerve (‘Tears’, 2018). Lacrimal gland secretion of proteins, electrolytes, and water is controlled by the activation of sympathetic and parasympathetic nerves that release neurotransmitters (Dartt, 2009). The key neurotransmitters in crying are norepinephrine and neuropeptide Y, which are sympathetic, and acetylcholine and vasoactive intestinal peptide, which are parasympathetic. All of these neurotransmitters stimulate separate signalling pathways, however these pathways do interact. Lastly, enkephalins, which are related to endorphins, activate an inhibitory pathway that blocks lacrimal gland secretion and crying (Dartt, 2009). | The trigeminal nerve, also known as the fifth cranial nerve, contains the sensory pathway that is responsible for tears (‘Tears’, 2018). The motor pathway is involuntary, and considered to be a part of the autonomic nervous system. This utilizes the pathway of the facial, or seventh cranial nerve (‘Tears’, 2018). Lacrimal gland secretion of proteins, electrolytes, and water is controlled by the activation of sympathetic and parasympathetic nerves that release neurotransmitters (Dartt, 2009). The key neurotransmitters in crying are norepinephrine and neuropeptide Y, which are sympathetic, and acetylcholine and vasoactive intestinal peptide, which are parasympathetic. All of these neurotransmitters stimulate separate signalling pathways, however these pathways do interact. Lastly, enkephalins, which are related to endorphins, activate an inhibitory pathway that blocks lacrimal gland secretion and crying (Dartt, 2009). | ||
+ | |||
+ | <box 50% round | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.26.00_pm.png?525 |}} </box| Figure 7: Neurologic system. https://study.com/academy/lesson/cranial-nerves-of-the-face-and-mouth-motion-and-sensation-functionality.html > | ||
====== Humans and Crying ====== | ====== Humans and Crying ====== | ||
- | <box 25% round right | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.30.42_pm.png?200|}} </box| Figure 5: The amygdala. https://www.neuroscientificallychallenged.com/blog/know-your-brain-amygdala | + | <box 30% round right | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.30.42_pm.png?250|}} </box| Figure 8: The amygdala. https://www.neuroscientificallychallenged.com/blog/know-your-brain-amygdala |
> | > | ||
Line 56: | Line 81: | ||
The amygdala is a structure within the brain that registers emotional reactions that in turn send its signals towards the hypothalamus (Lewis, 2016). The hypothalamus in turn activates the autonomic nervous system which proceeds to tear production (Lewis, 2016). The hypothalamus cannot tell what type of response is occurring, whether we are sad, happy, stressed, angry (Lewis, 2016) Basically, its role is to release the buildup of emotions that is occurring and it cannot distinguish between different types of emotions (Lewis, 2016). | The amygdala is a structure within the brain that registers emotional reactions that in turn send its signals towards the hypothalamus (Lewis, 2016). The hypothalamus in turn activates the autonomic nervous system which proceeds to tear production (Lewis, 2016). The hypothalamus cannot tell what type of response is occurring, whether we are sad, happy, stressed, angry (Lewis, 2016) Basically, its role is to release the buildup of emotions that is occurring and it cannot distinguish between different types of emotions (Lewis, 2016). | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
==== Why Do We Cry When We Are Happy? ==== | ==== Why Do We Cry When We Are Happy? ==== | ||
- | <box 25% round left | > {{:screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.35.29_pm.png?200|}} </box| Figure 6: Tears of grief. http://rose-lynnfisher.com/tears.html > | + | |
+ | <box 50% round | > {{ :crying_lebron.jpg?400 |}} </box| Figure 9: Tears of joy. https://www.esquire.com/sports/news/a45990/crying-lebron/ > | ||
+ | |||
It is an interesting concept to have negative emotion occur while you are happy. A group of researchers in Yale have figured out why exactly we cry tears of joy. Aragón et al., 2015 explained their results from their experiment in terms of a equilibrium of emotions. Individuals who participated in the study were presented with extremely positive emotions and their baseline emotional levels were recorded (Aragón et al., 2015). They wanted to see if there was a difference with the time it would take for individuals to return to this baseline emotional level and whether they used dimorphous expressions. The researchers found that individuals who responded to these extremely positive events with both a positive and negative emotion (crying) returned to their baseline emotional level quicker than participants who did not (Aragón et al., 2015). The researchers believe by responding to an overwhelmingly positive emotion with a negative one allows the individual to recover from strong emotions. Examples of this could be screaming while winning the lottery or crying at your child’s graduation (Aragón et al., 2015). | It is an interesting concept to have negative emotion occur while you are happy. A group of researchers in Yale have figured out why exactly we cry tears of joy. Aragón et al., 2015 explained their results from their experiment in terms of a equilibrium of emotions. Individuals who participated in the study were presented with extremely positive emotions and their baseline emotional levels were recorded (Aragón et al., 2015). They wanted to see if there was a difference with the time it would take for individuals to return to this baseline emotional level and whether they used dimorphous expressions. The researchers found that individuals who responded to these extremely positive events with both a positive and negative emotion (crying) returned to their baseline emotional level quicker than participants who did not (Aragón et al., 2015). The researchers believe by responding to an overwhelmingly positive emotion with a negative one allows the individual to recover from strong emotions. Examples of this could be screaming while winning the lottery or crying at your child’s graduation (Aragón et al., 2015). | ||
Line 65: | Line 95: | ||
==== Tear Composition ==== | ==== Tear Composition ==== | ||
- | |||
- | <box 25% round right | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.40.37_pm.png?250|}} </box| Figure 7: Laughing tears. http://rose-lynnfisher.com/tears.html > | ||
Your body produces 3 types of tears as stated above. The basal variety is used as a lubricant and protectant of the eye and coat your eyes each time you blink (Botelho, 1964). There are also reflex tears which are another form of protection released in response to wind, dirt etc… Finally, there are emotional tears (Botelho, 1964). Each of these types of tears is a combination of salt water, oils, antibodies and enzymes (Botelho, 1964). In the project topography of tears the photographer, Rose-Lynn Fisher examined dried human tears under a microscope to create a collection of different images. Her main conclusion was that tears coming from love developed patterns of complex beauty whereas tears coming from negative emotions became disordered (Fisher, 2017). | Your body produces 3 types of tears as stated above. The basal variety is used as a lubricant and protectant of the eye and coat your eyes each time you blink (Botelho, 1964). There are also reflex tears which are another form of protection released in response to wind, dirt etc… Finally, there are emotional tears (Botelho, 1964). Each of these types of tears is a combination of salt water, oils, antibodies and enzymes (Botelho, 1964). In the project topography of tears the photographer, Rose-Lynn Fisher examined dried human tears under a microscope to create a collection of different images. Her main conclusion was that tears coming from love developed patterns of complex beauty whereas tears coming from negative emotions became disordered (Fisher, 2017). | ||
+ | |||
+ | <box 25% round right | > {{ :screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.40.37_pm.png?250|}} </box| Figure 11: Laughing tears. http://rose-lynnfisher.com/tears.html > | ||
+ | |||
+ | <box 25% round left | > {{:screen_shot_2018-03-30_at_6.35.29_pm.png?250|}} </box| Figure 10: Tears of grief. http://rose-lynnfisher.com/tears.html > | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
Line 86: | Line 120: | ||
====== Psychopaths and Crying ====== | ====== Psychopaths and Crying ====== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | <box 50% round right | > {{ :hare_checklist.jpg?280 |}} </box| Figure 12: Hare checklist for psychopaths. https://thebrainofapsychopath.weebly.com/pathology-on-the-hare-psychopathy-checklist.html> | ||
+ | |||
Psychopaths make up approximately 1% of the population (Hare, 2003). They are individuals who unlike the rest of us act slightly differently and react to social situations in a unique way. Psychopaths are generally seen as individuals who do not have emotions, but rather are able to mimic the emotional response of others without truly feeling anything. The Hare checklist is used to diagnose an individual as a psychopath. Each person is given a rating of 0-2 for 20 items by a psychiatrist based on the degree to which they exhibit that behaviour (Hare, 2003). A few notable qualities are cunning and manipulative, glib and superficial charm, and criminal versatility. A study done by Rice et. al in 1992 looked at the likelihood of first time offenders to reoffend after being released from prison, based off of whether or not they were treated with empathy training. The participants were also rated on the Hare checklist and given a diagnosis of psychopathic or not. The results they found were quite disturbing—that is, individuals who were treated with empathy training who were not psychopaths showed a much lower number of re-offenses. On the contrary, individuals who were psychopaths who were treated with empathy training actually had a higher rate of re-offences. This suggests that psychopaths are able to learn from their environment and use emotions to manipulate the general population. | Psychopaths make up approximately 1% of the population (Hare, 2003). They are individuals who unlike the rest of us act slightly differently and react to social situations in a unique way. Psychopaths are generally seen as individuals who do not have emotions, but rather are able to mimic the emotional response of others without truly feeling anything. The Hare checklist is used to diagnose an individual as a psychopath. Each person is given a rating of 0-2 for 20 items by a psychiatrist based on the degree to which they exhibit that behaviour (Hare, 2003). A few notable qualities are cunning and manipulative, glib and superficial charm, and criminal versatility. A study done by Rice et. al in 1992 looked at the likelihood of first time offenders to reoffend after being released from prison, based off of whether or not they were treated with empathy training. The participants were also rated on the Hare checklist and given a diagnosis of psychopathic or not. The results they found were quite disturbing—that is, individuals who were treated with empathy training who were not psychopaths showed a much lower number of re-offenses. On the contrary, individuals who were psychopaths who were treated with empathy training actually had a higher rate of re-offences. This suggests that psychopaths are able to learn from their environment and use emotions to manipulate the general population. |